Driver Response to Active Front Steer and Power-Assist Failures

Similar documents
DRIVERS PERCEPTION OF AND RESPONSE TO BRAKE FAILURE

HOW DO DRIVERS BEHAVE IN A HIGHLY AUTOMATED CAR?

Design and Evaluation of Serial-Hybrid Vehicle Energy Gauges

MIT ICAT M I T I n t e r n a t i o n a l C e n t e r f o r A i r T r a n s p o r t a t i o n

DRIVING PERFORMANCE PROFILES OF DRIVERS WITH PARKINSON S DISEASE

STEERING ENTROPY AS A MEASURE OF IMPAIRMENT

Who has trouble reporting prior day events?

A Measuring Method for the Level of Consciousness while Driving Vehicles

Response to. Department for Transport Consultation Paper. Allowing Learner Drivers To Take Lessons on Motorways

Assessment of driver fitness: An alcohol calibration study in a high-fidelity simulation 26 April 2013

VINCI Autoroutes Foundation

Take-over time comparison by Demographics, Behavior, and Warning strength

The Effect of a Vehicle Control Device on Driver Performance in a Simulated Tank Driving Task

A fresh approach to the treatment of bends

Improvements to ramp metering system in England: VISSIM modelling of improvements

Evaluation of Perceptual Countermeasure Treatments Jemima Macaulay, Michael Tziotis (ARRB TR) Brian Fildes (MUARC)

Aria Etemad Volkswagen Group Research. Key Results. Aachen 28 June 2017

SUMMARY OF THE IMPACT ASSESSMENT

THE ACCELERATION OF LIGHT VEHICLES

Study of the Performance of a Driver-vehicle System for Changing the Steering Characteristics of a Vehicle

Use of Flow Network Modeling for the Design of an Intricate Cooling Manifold

Analyzing Crash Risk Using Automatic Traffic Recorder Speed Data

Non-contact Deflection Measurement at High Speed

Chapter 9 Real World Driving

Dynamic Behavior Analysis of Hydraulic Power Steering Systems

MOTORISTS' PREFERENCES FOR DIFFERENT LEVELS OF VEHICLE AUTOMATION

Road Safety s Mid Life Crisis The Trends and Characteristics for Middle Aged Controllers Involved in Road Trauma

Statistics and Facts About Distracted Driving

A KINEMATIC APPROACH TO HORIZONTAL CURVE TRANSITION DESIGN. James A. Bonneson, P.E.

Flagger Operations: Investigating Their Effectiveness in Capturing Driver Attention

Innovative Power Supply System for Regenerative Trains

Rural Speed and Crash Risk. Kloeden CN, McLean AJ Road Accident Research Unit, Adelaide University 5005 ABSTRACT

7. Author(s) Shan Bao, Michael J. Flannagan, James R. Sayer, Mitsuhiro Uchida 9. Performing Organization Name and Address

ROAD SAFETY RESEARCH, POLICING AND EDUCATION CONFERENCE, NOV 2001

Steer-by-Wire Systems with Integrated Torque Feedback Improve Steering Performance and Reduce Cost

A Preceding Vehicle Following System Based on Haptic Communication

2018 Linking Study: Predicting Performance on the Performance Evaluation for Alaska s Schools (PEAKS) based on MAP Growth Scores

Abstract. Executive Summary. Emily Rogers Jean Wang ORF 467 Final Report-Middlesex County

D-25 Speed Advisory System

A Practical Guide to Free Energy Devices

Implementation and Evaluation of Lane Departure Warning and Assistance Systems

2018 Linking Study: Predicting Performance on the NSCAS Summative ELA and Mathematics Assessments based on MAP Growth Scores

The Effects of Fatigue on Driver Performance for Single and Team Long-Haul Truck Drivers

2018 Linking Study: Predicting Performance on the TNReady Assessments based on MAP Growth Scores

Haptic Shared Control for Backward Parking and Its Effect on Skill Increase in Novice Drivers

DRIVER SPEED COMPLIANCE WITHIN SCHOOL ZONES AND EFFECTS OF 40 PAINTED SPEED LIMIT ON DRIVER SPEED BEHAVIOURS Tony Radalj Main Roads Western Australia

Sub-task B, Drivers needs to perceive the road at night by the use of road markings An experiment in the VTI driving simulator

MODELING SUSPENSION DAMPER MODULES USING LS-DYNA

WHAT IS THE PROFIT OF DRIVING FAST? -THE COMPARISON OF THE SPEEDY DRIVING AND SAFE DRIVING IN TERMS OF TRAVELING TIME-

The Impact of Sign Placement and Merge Type on Driving Behavior in Construction Zones

CHAPTER 6 MECHANICAL SHOCK TESTS ON DIP-PCB ASSEMBLY

Driver Acceptance and Use of a Speed Limit and Curve Advisor

Electromagnetic Fully Flexible Valve Actuator

VEHICLE AUTOMATION. CHALLENGES AND POTENTIAL FOR FUTURE MOBILITY.

Hardware Testing of Photovoltaic Inverter Loss of Mains Protection Performance

American Driving Survey,

Risk factors, driver behaviour and accident probability. The case of distracted driving.

VEHICLE DYNAMICS. A factsheet on Volvo Cars Scalable Product Architecture chassis technology

The World s Best Driving Road

MOTORISTS' PREFERENCES FOR DIFFERENT LEVELS OF VEHICLE AUTOMATION: 2016

Extracting Tire Model Parameters From Test Data

AdaptIVe: Automated driving applications and technologies for intelligent vehicles

Magnus HJÄLMDAHL Sverker ALMQVIST András VÁRHELYI

2018 AER Social Research Report

White paper: Pneumatics or electrics important criteria when choosing technology

JCE 4600 Basic Freeway Segments

Minnesota Mileage-Based User Fee Test Results. Ray Starr Office of Traffic, Safety and Technology Minnesota Department of Transportation

Traffic and Toll Revenue Estimates

SAE Mini BAJA: Suspension and Steering

Post 50 km/h Implementation Driver Speed Compliance Western Australian Experience in Perth Metropolitan Area

QuickStick Repeatability Analysis

Effect of Police Control on U-turn Saturation Flow at Different Median Widths

Using cloud to develop and deploy advanced fault management strategies

Four Wheel Steering Mechanism

EVALUATION OF ACCIDENT AVOIDANCE SUPPORTING SYSTEM AT INTERSECTIONS FOR MOTORCYCLISTS USING ADAS

Enhancing Wheelchair Mobility Through Dynamics Mimicking

REAL-WORLD BENEFITS OF ADAPTIVE HEADLIGHTS (ADHL) ON PASSENGER CARS IN SWEDEN

WHITE PAPER. Preventing Collisions and Reducing Fleet Costs While Using the Zendrive Dashboard

Active Driver Assistance for Vehicle Lanekeeping

ANALYSIS OF GEAR QUALITY CRITERIA AND PERFORMANCE OF CURVED FACE WIDTH SPUR GEARS

METHOD FOR TESTING STEERABILITY AND STABILITY OF MILITARY VEHICLES MOTION USING SR60E STEERING ROBOT

Mac McCall VTTI Motorcycle Research Group September 28, 2017

Oregon DOT Slow-Speed Weigh-in-Motion (SWIM) Project: Analysis of Initial Weight Data

30 top tips to tackle HVAC challenges No.09 - Energy efficiency

Fleet Penetration of Automated Vehicles: A Microsimulation Analysis

Multi Body Dynamic Analysis of Slider Crank Mechanism to Study the effect of Cylinder Offset

Design and development of mobile service for ecodriving

Vehicle Dynamics and Drive Control for Adaptive Cruise Vehicles

The Study of Locomotion of Small Wheeled Rovers: The MIDD Activity

Relationship between steering torque and ease of driving with bar type steering in high speed range

AN ANALYSIS OF DRIVER S BEHAVIOR AT MERGING SECTION ON TOKYO METOPOLITAN EXPRESSWAY WITH THE VIEWPOINT OF MIXTURE AHS SYSTEM

FMVSS 121 Brake Performance and Stability Testing

Surface- and Pressure-Dependent Characterization of SAE Baja Tire Rolling Resistance

Missouri Seat Belt Usage Survey for 2017

ISA: The Research Evidence

E61, E63, E64, E70, E81, E87, E90, E91, E92, E93 BMW AG - TIS

Investigation in to the Application of PLS in MPC Schemes

Level of Service Classification for Urban Heterogeneous Traffic: A Case Study of Kanapur Metropolis

Keywords: driver support and platooning, yaw stability, closed loop performance

WP6. DELIVERABLE HYTEC PRE-TRIAL SURVEYS

Transcription:

Driver Response to Active Front Steer and Power-Assist Failures Hamish Jamson*, Phil Wiffin and Peter Burchill *Institute for Transport Studies, University of Leeds, LS2 9JT. Jaguar Research Abstract Active Front Steer (AFS) has the ability to apply varying road wheel angles for the same steering wheel angle, depending on the speed of the vehicle. It achieves this by changing the steering gain dynamically and can be a useful driver aid: less steering wheel input is required at low speeds than a more traditional fixedgain steering system to achieve the same radius of turn. Should an AFS system fail, it is designed to revert back to a fixed-gain system. Concerns have been expressed by designers of AFS systems that this sudden change in steering gain may prove hazardous for drivers and be particularly difficult for them to handle. The present study had three main aims: To compare drivers behaviour using both fully-functioning AFS and power-assisted, fixed-gain, rack-and-pinion steering systems. To compare driver behaviour and response to the failures of both AFS and to the power-assist of a fixed-gain, rack-and-pinion steering system. To conclude whether failure of AFS is potentially more hazardous than failure of the power-assist of fixed ratio, rack-and-pinion steering systems. Using the fixed-base Leeds Driving Simulator, this study compared driver behaviour using both AFS and fixed-gain steering systems. Failures of each system were also investigated. Fixed-gain system failure was simulated by a loss of steering power-assist. Forty drivers, balanced for age and gender took part in the study. Results showed that fully-functioning AFS had some advantages over fully-functioning fixed-gain steering. Drivers found AFS less demanding, demonstrated by the fact that they showed significantly fewer steering reversals. They also rated AFS as easier to control in curved sections. Whilst making left-hand turns at a series of T-shaped intersections, there was a highly significant worsening of driver performance between steering functioning normally and failed steering. However, drivers found AFS failure no harder to manage than power-assist failure. Indeed, there were statistical trends suggesting that, if anything, AFS failure was easier for drivers to deal with than loss of power-assist. 98

Introduction The function of a steering system of a vehicle is to steer the front wheels in response to driver commands in order to provide effective direction control. The large majority of the current vehicle fleet uses a simple rack-and-pinion steering system, where driver rotational inputs to the steering wheel are transformed by the pinion to a translational motion of the steering rack. The steering rack is connected to the steering arm by a tierod, the connection controlling the steering angle proportional to the rack movement. AFS is a system pioneered by BMW (Köhn et al., 2002; Krenn and Richter, 2004) and is currently available as an option for the first time on the new BMW 5 series. The system is made up of a rack-and-pinion steering system, a double planetary gear and an electrical actuator motor. By adjusting the steering gain (rack travel versus steering input), the system has the ability to modify the driver-demanded road wheel angle at the input to the steering rack, depending on the speed of the vehicle. AFS can be a useful driver aid: less steering wheel input is required at low speeds than a more traditional fixed-gain steering system to achieve the same radius of turn. Should an AFS system fail, it is designed to revert back to a fixed-gain system. Concerns have been expressed by designers of AFS systems that this sudden change in steering gain may prove hazardous for drivers and be particularly difficult for them to handle. By observing driver behaviour in the Leeds Driving Simulator, the present study, funded by Jaguar Cars, had three main aims: To compare drivers behaviour using both fully-functioning AFS and power-assisted, fixed-gain, rack-and-pinion steering systems. To compare driver behaviour and response to the failures of both AFS and to the power-assist of a fixed-gain, rack-and-pinion steering system. To conclude whether failure of AFS is potentially more hazardous than failure of the power-assist of fixed ratio, rack-and-pinion steering systems. Methodology Participants 40 drivers took part in the study. Experienced simulator drivers were recruited with a stipulation that they had at least two years driving experience and drove at least 4000 miles per annum. The sample was balanced for gender and age (under 35 and over 35) giving four groups: young male, young female, old male, old female. The demographics of the groups are show in Table 1 along with their means and standard deviations (SD). Drivers were paid for their participation in the study. 99

Driver group Age Driving experience Annual mileage Young male 28.1 years, SD = 3.3 9.4 years, SD = 3.9 12800 miles, SD = 10623 Young female 27.1 years, SD = 4.0 9.2 years, SD = 4.3 8800 miles, SD = 5094 Old male 53.5 years, SD = 14.7 33.6 years, SD = 13.6 11400 miles, SD = 7575 Old female 55.4 years, SD = 17.5 27.1 years, SD = 14.1 8350 miles, SD = 4056 Table 1: demographics of the participant driving sample Driving Simulator The study was performed using scenarios created in the Leeds Driving Simulator (Figure 1). The simulator is currently a fixed-based facility, built around a complete Rover 216GTi with its driver controls and dashboard instrumentation fully operational. The projection system consists of five forward channels, the images edge-blended to provide a near seamless horizontal field of view of 230. A rear view (60 ) is back projected onto a screen behind the car to provide an image seen through the vehicle's rear view mirror. The frame rate is fixed to a constant 60Hz. The simulation uses naturalistic scenery modelled using MultiGen-Paradigm Creator and operates with in-house code to model realistic vehicle dynamics and intelligent scenario control to choreograph specific traffic events. Although the simulator is fixed-base, torque feedback at the steering wheel is provided via a motor fixed at the end of the steering column and a vacuum motor provides the brake pedal booster assistance. Data are collected at the frame rate. Figure 1: The Leeds Driving Simulator Experimental design There were two main experimental factors under investigation: steering type (AFS, power-assisted fixed-gain) steering failure (steering functioning normally, steering failed) 100

The study was designed as within-subjects, such that each driver experienced both types of steering system, both functioning normally and under failure. Steering failures occurred at give-way intersections where drivers were required to make a left-hand turn. Steering type The two steering types were modelled in the simulator. The AFS system was simulated 80 according to Burchill (2003), 75 allowing a variable steering gain 70 65 over a speed range from rest to 60 100mph (Figure 2). The powerassisted, 55 rack-and-pinion system 50 had a fixed steering gain of 45 40mm/rev, equating to a steering 40 ratio of steering wheel to road 35 wheel movement of around 18:1. AFS Steering Gain (mm/rev) 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 Vehicle Speed (mph) Figure 2: change in AFS steering gain with vehicle speed Steering failure For the AFS system, the variable steering gain that was apparent before failure was fixed at 40mm/rev after failure. Power-assistance to the steering was still available. The effect to the driver, particularly at low speed, was that larger than expected steering wheel input was required in order to negotiate a turn of fixed radius. For the fixed-gain system, failure constituted of a loss of power-assist. The effect to the driver was that the expected steering angle was still required, but much larger effort was required to rotate the steering wheel in order to achieve a fixed radius turn. Virtual road network Participant drivers drove the same experimental road network three times. The road network was about 8km long with a 96kph (60mph) posted speed limit and took around 8 minutes to complete. There were two 3.65m wide lanes, one in each direction with no verge nor shoulder to the lane. There were three T-shaped intersections in the road network and drivers were given automated auditory instructions to turn left at each of these junctions. The surrounding virtual environment mimicked a rural road layout with medium density, on-coming traffic. Participant drivers were instructed to drive as naturally as possible, bearing in mind the speed limit of the rural road. Experimental procedure Data collection took around one hour per driver. Participants drove the simulator five times during their visit. Firstly, they drove a practice road network in whichever condition of steering type they had first been allocated. The order in which steering type was experienced was counterbalanced, such that half of the drivers participated with AFS first followed by fixed-gain steering and half with fixed-gain followed by AFS. During the first practice session, driver re-familiarised themselves with the handling of the 101

simulator over around 10km of winding, virtual road at around 50-60mph driving speed. They also practised negotiation of ninety-degree, left-hand turns at number of T-shaped intersections; at least six practice manoeuvres were performed. After practicing at both low and high speeds with a given steering type, data collection began during the second drive. The three T-shaped intersections of the experimental road network ( Figure 3) were each separated by around 2km. The separating sections were both straight and curved. A straight section was 864m in length (30s of driving at 60mph). A curved section of roadway, also 864m, was made up of 18 curved segments making a double s- shaped bend. Curves varied left and right, radius fluctuated between 510m and 750m. This gentle curving scenario required some negotiation by the driver and workload was considered to be higher than the simple straight sections. Figure 3: intersection negotiation and on-coming vehicles During this second drive, the vehicle s steering performed normally up until the third and final junction. When the vehicle was 30m away from this intersection, the steering failed. The experimental run concluded when the intersection had been negotiated and the vehicle was at least 100m along the next road segment. Oncoming vehicles at the intersections motivated drivers to return to an accurate lane position as soon as possible ( Figure 3). For the third drive, steering type was changed from either AFS to fixed-gain or vice versa. This drive was another familiarisation drive, exactly the same as the first drive, except with the alternate steering type. Again, at least six practice intersection negotiations were performed. Data collection for the alternate steering type was made during the fourth drive. This was identical to the second drive, except that there were no steering failures. This was because 102

it was felt that after the second drive, participants may have begun to associate the third intersection with a steering failure. In order to minimise these learning effects and to keep each steering failure as unexpected as possible, the failure of the alternate steering type occurred on the fifth and final drive, during the first intersection encountered. Results With fully-functioning steering, repeated-measures ANOVA were used. The two withinsubject factors were scenario (two levels: straight and curved section) and steering type (two levels: AFS and fixed gain (FG) steering). Normality and sphericity tests performed to ensure that the assumptions of ANOVA were not violated. For the analysis of steering failures, only the intersection scenarios were used since failures only occurred at these locations. Again, repeated-measures ANOVA were used but since the two steering types were quite different in their respective failure mode, a single within-subject factor design was employed: steering condition (AFS, fixed gain (FG), AFS fail, power-assist fail). Driver self-reported ratings Ratings were made on how easy the participants found the control of the vehicle with its current steering system. Participants were required to rate the steering system on a scale from 0 to 10 (low score = difficulty to control). steering rating (out of 10) Fully-functioning steering The mean of all ratings made for straight and curved section for both steering types are shown in Figure 4. There was a main effect 8.3 of scenario in that steering in curves was 8.1 rated as more difficult than in straights, 7.9 7.7 F(1,39)=27.9, p<.001. There was no effect 7.5 AFS of steering type (F(1,39) = 0.46, p=.50) but 7.3 FG the interaction almost reached significance, 7.1 F(1,39)=3.43, p=.072. This interaction 6.9 suggests that FG was rated much lower 6.7 than AFS on curves than on straights. 6.5 straight curve Figure 4: steering rating for AFS and fixed gain steering (FG) on straight and curved sections steering rating (out of 10) Steering failure 8 7.5 7 6.5 6 5.5 5 AFS FG A comparison of ratings was also made at intersections with both types of steering type both functioning normally and failed (Figure 5). There was a main effect of steering failure with drivers rating failed steering much lower than fullyfunctioning steering, F(1,39)=57.2, 4.5 no failure failure 103

p<.001. There was no effect of steering type (F(1,39) = 1.37, p=.25) but the interaction again almost reached significance, F(1,39)=3.49, p=.069. Considering the no failure condition alone, AFS was rated higher than FG, but this effect did not quite reach significance, F(1,39)=3.60, p=.065. Figure 5: steering rating for normally functioning and failed AFS and FG at intersections Steering reversal rate The number of 1º steering reversals per minute (McLean & Hoffmann, 1975), steering reversal rate, was recorded over each curve and straight section. Mean values are shown in Figure 6. As with previous measures of lateral performance, the number of steering reversals was significantly lower on 30 straights and than curves, 25 F(1,39)=165, p<.001. There was also a main effect of steering type, in 20 that there were significantly higher AFS 15 FG reversal rates with drivers using the 10 fixed gain steering as opposed to AFS, F(1,39)=5.15, p=.029. There 5 was no interaction between scenario 0 and steering type, F(1,39)=.07, straight curve p=.79. steering reversal rate (per minute) Figure 6: steering reversal rate for AFS and FG on straight and curved sections Time spent out of lane As the lane position achieved by the outside of each offside wheel was recorded during intersection negotiation, it was also possible to measure the time spent with any part of the vehicle outside of its lane boundary, including an inferred lane boundary during intersection negotiation. The red hatched area of Figure 7 shows the inferred lane boundary. Time spent out of this area during intersection negotiation was defined as time spent out of lane. Figure 7: intersection inferred lane boundary (denoted by red hatched area) time spenf out of lane (s) 3.5 3 2.5 2 1.5 1 0.5 0 AFS FG AFS fail power-assist fail 104 There was a strong main effect of steering condition, F(3,117)=34.2, p<.001. Post-hoc tests (Tukey LSD) showed that in both failed states, drivers spent longer periods whilst encroaching into the adjacent lane than with steering

functioning normally (AFS-AFS fail, p<.001; AFS-power assist failure, p<.001; FG-AFS fail, p<.001; FG-power assist fail, p<.001). There was a trend suggesting that in fullyfunctioning steering conditions, drivers encroached for shorter durations with AFS than with FG (p=.095) and also with AFS failed compared to assistance failure (p=.15). Figure 8: time spent out of lane during/after intersection for all steering conditions Discussion This investigation examined the effects of and differences between an Active Front Steer (AFS) and a power-assisted, fixed-gain steering system on driver behaviour. In order to assess the effects of each individual steering system, behaviour was recorded with systems functioning normally on straight and curved sections of roadway. Drivers also encountered several T-shaped intersections both with functioning and failed steering to evaluate the severity of such failures. The study took place using the Leeds Driving Simulator. At first, drivers experienced fully-operational steering systems on a single-carriageway, rural road with a posted speed limit of 60mph. Driver had to negotiate both straight and curved sections of virtual road. On average, drivers rated AFS higher, in that they found it easier to position their vehicle in lane with this system as opposed to fixed-gain steering. However, this was a trend only on curved sections of road; there was statistically no difference in ratings on straight sections. This result is probably not too surprising as straight sections, in general, require less driver interaction in order to negotiate effectively. This easy driving has probably been shown as a floor effect in driver ratings. Since negotiating a straight is more straight-forward than negotiating a curve, the subtle difference between steering systems is potentially lost. Since they rated AFS higher on curves, drivers were likely to feel more confident with that system. This may be the reason that they tended to drive slightly quicker with AFS. However, this result was not statistically significant and thus no compensatory effect (increased speed due to less demanding steering controls) was proven. This is promising as no vehicle designer would want to loose a safety benefit (increased speed) to gain a safety benefit (less demanding steering). Lateral control measures tended to back up drivers reported preference of AFS. Drivers had significantly fewer steering reversals, suggesting less mental effort was required in maintaining accurate steering. Drivers also showed significantly less variation in steering wheel movement. Partially due to a system effect (AFS at these speeds requires around 11% less steering input from the driver), less steering wheel variation is also associated with a less demanding steering task. Whilst the type of steering system had little effect of variation in lane position, the more sensitive measure of time to line crossing also showed trends towards a safety benefit of AFS, particularly on curved sections of road. Whilst these fully-functioning steering results are interesting, the main aim of the study was to assess the severity of AFS failure compared with failure in the power-assist of the 105

fixed-gain steering system. All of the driving measures employed during intersection negotiation were recorded between the start and end of intersection negotiation. Start was defined as the time at which the centre of gravity of the vehicle crossed the dashed giveway line of the junction. End occurred when the driver managed to straighten up the vehicle, characterised as achieving a heading error of less than 1 for at least 2s and was the initial time frame of this 2s period. Whilst drivers took significantly longer to regain control of the vehicle (end time start time) with a steering failure than with the corresponding steering functioning normally, there was no observed difference between the two full-functioning steering conditions (AFS and fixed-gain) nor between failed steering conditions (AFS failure and power-assist failure). As drivers always made a left-hand turn at intersections, a lane boundary could be inferred during the left-turn and then again when joining the main traffic stream (Figure 7). Between the start and end of intersection negotiation, the maximum lane position achieved, i.e. the largest distance from the offside of the vehicle to the lane boundary, was much greater during a steering failure than with steering fully-operational. However, as in the case of time to regain control, there was no established difference for drivers experiencing AFS failure and when they underwent a failure of power-assist. However, in terms of time spent out of the inferred lane boundary, drivers tended to encroach into the adjacent lane for longer periods with fixed-gain steering than with AFS. This may due to the fact that there is a lesser steering wheel input requirement with AFS. Drivers also tended to encroach marginally further during power-assist failure than during AFS failure. Conclusions In summary, some advantages of fully functioning AFS over fully-functioning fixed-gain steering were demonstrated. Drivers found the steering less demanding (fewer steering reversals) and rated AFS easier to control in curved sections. Whilst all of the driving measures employed during intersection negotiation showed a worsening of driver performance between steering functioning normally and failed steering, it appeared that it was no harder for drivers to manage AFS failure than power-assist failure. In fact, there were statistical trends suggesting that, if anything, AFS failure was less disturbing than power-assist failure. References Burchill, P. (2003). Simple X202 Steering Model: Production Version. Internal Jaguar Report. Köhn, P., Baumgarten, G., Richter, T., Schuster, M. and Fleck, M. (2002). The Active Steering System: the New Dynamic Road Handling and Steering System from BMW. 11 th Aachen vehicle and engine technology colloquium, Aachen, 2002. 106

Krenn, M. and Richter, T. (2004). Active steering BMW s approach to modern steering technology. In: Barton, D.C. and Blackwood, A (Eds.): Braking 2004. Vehicle Braking and Chassis Control. pp 3-14. McLean, J.R. and Hoffmann, E.R. (1975). Steering reversals as a measure of driver performance and steering task difficulty. Human Factors, 17, 248-256. 107